MAR 20, 2015 12:54 PM PDT

"Winner Effect" of Kill Tempered by Oxytocin in Hunters

WRITTEN BY: Will Hector
When Tsimane men come home to their families after a day of hunting, their levels of oxytocin-the "love hormone"-go up.

The Tsimane are an indigenous population of forager-farmers and hunters who live in the lowlands of Bolivia's Amazon basin. The human hormone system should be particularly well adapted to their lifestyle-small, tight-knit communities that produce their own food.

"Our goal was to look at the interaction between different hormones in motivating behavior in a naturalistic context," says Adrian Jaeggi, a postdoctoral scholar of anthropology at the University of California, Santa Barbara, and co-lead author of the new study.

"Hunting for subsistence and sharing meat is something people have done for hundreds of thousands of years."

Previous studies have shown that oxytocin makes people more cooperative while testosterone has the opposite effect. Jaeggi says he expected to see a trade-off between the two hormones and was surprised to find the positive relationship between them.

High testosterone while hunting can be attributed to a "winner effect" experienced by men making a kill. It can also be related to the status competition that hunting represents for traditional societies such as the Tsimane.

In either case, the concurrently increased oxytocin could serve as a balance to make the hunters kinder, more generous, and more willing to share their bounty.

"Almost half a century ago, it was famously documented that successful !Kung hunters were jokingly insulted by others in order to ‘cool their hearts' to ‘make (them) gentle,' lest pride or boasting disrupt the egalitarian social system common to many foragers," says Michael Gurven, professor of anthropology, co-director of the Tsimane Health and Life History Project, and senior coauthor of the study published in the journal Biology Letters.

"Here we observe a potential hormonal analog consistent with the type of leveling behavior seen in hunter-gatherer societies."

An additional function of both hormones may have to do with post-hunt regeneration. Both are shown to assist with muscle rebuilding following physical activity.

"These men are coming home, they're finished with work for the day, and they're about to eat and share food," Jaeggi says. "So the need to be social coincides with the need to regenerate and it would make sense for the same hormones to facilitate both functions."

To conduct the study, co-lead author Ben Trumble joined the hunters as they went out into the jungle and made a kill-or didn't-and returned home with-or without-food for their families.

He took saliva specimens along the way, collecting them at exact intervals. The oxytocin was measured in the UCSB Human Biodemography Laboratory.

Though the sample size is "not huge," the researchers note the study "definitely adds to the current literature in which the interplay between testosterone and oxytocin is often overlooked."

More than anything else, the sample size is a function of the cost of the lab assays needed to measure the oxytocin. "One of our plans is to develop our own assay to make it less expensive so we can analyze much larger samples," Jaeggi says.

So how can the findings among a group of indigenous hunter-gatherers in central Bolivia be applied to modern Western culture?

"I think the ‘absence makes the heart grow fonder' effect could potentially be very widespread," Jaeggi says.

"Reconnecting with their families after a day of separation would have been a very common challenge for men throughout evolutionary history, and oxytocin could help with that. Another interesting correlation, Trumble says, is that the average Tsimane hunt lasts eight and a half hours, roughly equivalent to a workday here.

Previous research on oxytocin has shown its value as a means of quantifying the depth of interpersonal relationships, Jaeggi says.

"Oxytocin levels indicate how much you value another person. It's like a physiological measure of the value of that relationship."

Mothers and infants

For example, fall in love with someone and your oxytocin level will skyrocket whenever your sweetheart is near, both literally and figuratively. "Even talking to someone on the phone is enough to cause that oxytocin increase," he says.

Men are not the sole beneficiaries of oxytocin's positive effects.

"Like the biology of most social behaviors, it originated in the mother-infant context, where it facilitates giving birth, nursing and bonding," Jaeggi says. "It is found in all mammals, and it then got used in other contexts in some species."

One finding that surprised the researchers involved the correlation of social variables to oxytocin increase: There was none.

"I would have expected that men who have more children experience a greater increase when they come home because they're about to see their families," Jaeggi says. "Or the guys who encounter people on the way home and get to show off their hunting skills-none of these things had an effect on the oxytocin change. The magnitude of the increase didn't differ in relation to these social variables."

The researchers are careful to point out that an inherent problem in their research has to do with the fact that oxytocin has two distinct release systems: one in the brain and one in the rest of the body. Therefore, their measure of oxytocin circulating in the blood may not correspond to levels in the brain.

"Some of the social effects of oxytocin might be happening in the brain and we aren't necessarily capturing that with the saliva sample," Jaeggi says. "Of course in this kind of study, all we can do is measure peripheral oxytocin. Measuring oxytocin in the brain would be highly invasive."

Oxytocin studies in the United States often make use of nasal sprays to administer the hormone, Trumble says. "There's a set of tissues right where arteries meet the brain called the blood-brain barrier.

Any molecule above a certain size can't pass through it. But there's a weakness in the blood-brain barrier in the nasal canal and allows some transfer to occur. So many experimental studies of oxytocin don't actually measure oxytocin levels at all, but take instead administer oxytocin and examine before-and-after changes in behavior."

Source: University of California--Santa Barbara
About the Author
Bachelor's (BA/BS/Other)
Will Hector practices psychotherapy at Heart in Balance Counseling Center in Oakland, California. He has substantial training in Attachment Theory, Hakomi Body-Centered Psychotherapy, Psycho-Physical Therapy, and Formative Psychology. To learn more about his practice, click here: http://www.heartinbalancetherapy.com/will-hector.html
You May Also Like
Loading Comments...